http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00152/full?
- 1Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France
- 2CNRS, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, UMR 8242, Paris, France
- 3Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland
Introduction
Recent theories of sensory processing consider the brain
as a proactive system which adapts quickly to the environment. Neurons
in the sensory cortices can undergo short-term, task-dependent, and
context-specific changes in receptive field properties when attention
and prediction are involved (Fritz et al., 2003, 2007, 2008).
Such adaptive plasticity driven by attention and prediction can be the
underlying mechanism for the optimization of perception.
Attention is suggested to have a global effect on
perception at an early stage of sensory processing.
Electroencephalography (EEG) studies revealed the neuronal consequences
of attention on event-related potentials (ERPs), particularly the
enhancement of the N1 (Hillyard et al., 1973; Alcaini et al., 1994; Lange et al., 2003, 2006; Lange and Röder, 2006). This may result from changes in the selectivity of neurons in the sensory cortex (Chawla et al., 1999; Kastner et al., 1999; Ahveninen et al., 2006).
Specifically, research showing that the auditory N1 response is
modulated by task demands and notched-noise masking suggests that the
spectrotemporal receptive fields of neurons are tuned according to
attentional manipulations (Kauramäki et al., 2007), as attention excites neurons responsive to attended features and inhibits neurons responsive to unattended features (Fritz et al., 2003, 2007, 2008; Jääskeläinen et al., 2007).
Neurocomputational studies demonstrated that attention may function via
optimizing the synaptic gain to represent the precision of sensory
information during hierarchical inference (Feldman and Friston, 2010).
Prediction, or the statistical regularity in the
environment, is also suggested to modulate the early stage of sensory
processing, albeit its effect on ERPs manifests as a suppression of the
N1 (Lange, 2013). Prediction-related N1 suppression was demonstrated when participants had foreknowledge of the upcoming stimuli (Schafer and Marcus, 1973; Schafer et al., 1981; Lange, 2009; SanMiguel et al., 2013; Timm et al., 2013).
The predictive coding model postulates that the prediction effect
indexes the difference in neurocomputational demand for
predictable/unpredictable information (Friston, 2005; Egner et al., 2010).
Specifically, it indexes how much of the sensory input cannot be
accounted for by the internal model. Moreover, prediction was reported
to alter the contrast gain of sensory evidence accumulation (Melloni et al., 2011; Rohenkohl et al., 2012).
Neurophysiologically, this is reflected in sharper sensory
representations where the reduction of neuronal activity is smaller in
neurons responsive to predictable features than in neurons responsive to
unpredictable features (Kok et al., 2012a).
Despite their ERP effects being opposite, the relation
of attention and prediction remains undetermined. This might be due to
the conflation of these two mechanisms in the literature, where
attention and prediction were often treated as the same concept (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). However, attention and prediction can rely on orthogonal sources of information (Summerfield and Egner, 2013). While attention operates on the basis of motivational relevance, prediction operates on the basis of prior likelihood (Summerfield and Egner, 2009). It is possible that attention and prediction are two independent mechanisms which may have antagonistic (Summerfield and Egner, 2009) or additive (Timm et al., 2013)
effects on neuronal signals for sensory processing. Alternatively,
attention and prediction may be dependent of each other. One possibility
is that one of the two mechanisms is necessary for the other to take
effect, but not the other way round (Kok et al., 2012b).
Another possibility is that such dependency is bidirectional, with both
attention and prediction being necessary to modulate sensory
processing.
To examine the relation between these two top-down
mechanisms, we orthogonally manipulated attention and prediction in a
target detection task. Participants were instructed to pay attention to
one of two interleaved stimulus streams of predictable/unpredictable
tone frequency. Using EEG, we quantified N1 and P2 as dependent
variables given that the former is involved in auditory perception and
the latter is suggested to reflect the comparison between the sensory
input and the internal model (Evans and Federmeier, 2007; Costa-Faidella et al., 2011).
The design allowed us to evaluate whether attention and prediction are
dependent of each other, and if so, how these two top-down mechanisms
may interact on sensory processing.
Figures and more at the link.
No comments:
Post a Comment