Possible benefits for TBI, need research for stroke.
You'll have to look at Table 1 in the PDF to see what testing results have shown, Figure 1 at the bottom is interesting on how creatine gets in the brain.
Creatine Supplementation and Brain Health
Hamilton Roschel 1,* , Bruno Gualano 1,2, Sergej M. Ostojic 3 and Eric S. Rawson 4
Citation: Roschel, H.; Gualano, B.;
Ostojic, S.M.; Rawson, E.S. Creatine
Supplementation and Brain Health.
Nutrients 2021, 13, 586. https://
doi.org/10.3390/nu13020586
Academic Editor: Richard B. Kreider
Received: 18 January 2021
Accepted: 4 February 2021
Published: 10 February 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1 Applied Physiology & Nutrition Research Group, Rheumatology Division, School of Physical Education
and Sport, Faculdade de Medicina FMUSP, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo 01246-903, Brazil;
gualano@usp.br
2 Food Research Center, University of São Paulo, Sao Paulo 05508-080, Brazil
3 FSPE Applied Bioenergetics Lab, University of Novi Sad, 21000 Novi Sad, Serbia; sergej.ostojic@chess.edu.rs
4 Department of Health, Nutrition, and Exercise Science, Messiah University, Mechanicsburg, PA 17055, USA;
erawson@messiah.edu
* Correspondence: hars@usp.br; Tel.: +55-11-3061-8789
Abstract:
There is a robust and compelling body of evidence supporting the ergogenic and therapeutic
role of creatine supplementation in muscle. Beyond these well-described effects and mechanisms,
there is literature to suggest that creatine may also be beneficial to brain health (e.g., cognitive
processing, brain function, and recovery from trauma). This is a growing field of research, and
the purpose of this short review is to provide an update on the effects of creatine supplementation
on brain health in humans. There is a potential for creatine supplementation to improve cognitive
processing, especially in conditions characterized by brain creatine deficits, which could be induced
by acute stressors (e.g., exercise, sleep deprivation) or chronic, pathologic conditions (e.g., creatine
synthesis enzyme deficiencies, mild traumatic brain injury, aging, Alzheimer’s disease, depression).
Despite this, the optimal creatine protocol able to increase brain creatine levels is still to be determined.
Similarly, supplementation studies concomitantly assessing brain creatine and cognitive function are
needed. Collectively, data available are promising and future research in the area is warranted.
Keywords:
phosphorylcreatine; dietary supplement; cognition; brain injury; concussion
1. Introduction
The ergogenic effects of creatine supplementation are well documented, with evidence supporting its efficacy in increasing muscle strength, lean mass, and exercise performance/muscle function, particularly when combined with exercise in different populations,
from athletes to a wide spectrum of patient populations [1–3].
Creatine mechanisms of action involve rapid energy provision by transferring the
N-phosphoryl group from phosphorylcreatine (PCr) to adenosine diphosphate (ADP),
thus resynthesizing adenosine triphosphate (ATP) and spatial energy buffering, transferring energy from the mitochondria to the cytosol. These mechanisms are responsible for
facilitating ATP homeostasis during high energy turnover, maintaining a low ADP concentration and reducing Ca2+ leakage from the sarcoplasmic reticulum and impairment of
force output of the muscle [4–6]. Additionally, creatine could also attenuate the formation
of reactive oxygen species by its coupling with ATP into the mitochondria or by scavenging
radical species in an acellular setting [7]. Its direct and indirect antioxidant effects have
been suggested to have therapeutic effects in neurodegenerative diseases [8].
Although most of the total body’s creatine is found in skeletal muscle, the brain is
also a very metabolically active tissue, accounting for up to 20% of the body’s energy
consumption [9,10]. Creatine kinase (CK), a main enzyme involved in the ATP/CK/PCr
system, is also expressed in a brain-specific isoform (BB-CK) [4–6], suggesting that creatine
may also be relevant for energy provision to the central nervous system (CNS). In fact,
creatine-deficient syndromes involving brain creatine depletion are characterized by major
mental and developmental disorders (e.g., mental retardation, learning delays, autism, and which may be partially reversed by creatine supplementation [11–14]. Cognitive
processing may also be affected by creatine metabolism, as it may facilitate ATP homeostasis
during periods of rapid or altered brain ATP turnover, such as during complex cognitive
tasks, hypoxia, sleep deprivation, and some neurological conditions [3,15,16]. Additionally,
creatine supplementation might be beneficial for mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI),
which is also associated with changes in brain energy needs. The effects of creatine
supplementation on brain creatine levels, cognitive processing, and mTBI have been
previously reviewed [3,17,18]. As this is a growing field, the purpose of this short review is
to provide an update regarding the effects of creatine supplementation on brain health in
humans beyond what is discussed in Dolan et al. [3].
2. The Effects of Creatine Supplementation on Brain Creatine Levels
While muscle exclusively relies on dietary ingestion and endogenous synthesis from
the liver, kidneys, and pancreas [19], the brain can synthesize creatine. The enzymatic
apparatus necessary for endogenous creatine synthesis is found in the nervous system and
creatine transporters are found at the blood–brain barrier, neurons and oligodendrocytes
cells, indicating that brain creatine may not solely dependent on endogenous production
from other organs or dietary sources [20]. Furthermore, brain creatine seems not to be
influenced by habitual dietary intake from food, as similar brain PCr is found between
vegetarians and omnivores [21]. Still, if the intracerebral synthesis is limited due to
inherited disorders of creatine-catalyzing enzyme(s) machinery, dietary provision of the
compound can positively affect brain creatine concentrations [22]. Figure 1 illustrates
endogenous creatine synthesis in the brain and its transport across the blood–brain barrier.
2 of 10
creatine-deficient syndromes involving brain creatine depletion are characterized by major mental and developmental disorders (e.g., mental retardation, learning delays, autism,
and seizures), which may be partially reversed by creatine supplementation [11–14]. Cognitive processing may also be affected by creatine metabolism, as it may facilitate ATP
homeostasis during periods of rapid or altered brain ATP turnover, such as during complex cognitive tasks, hypoxia, sleep deprivation, and some neurological conditions
[3,15,16]. Additionally, creatine supplementation might be beneficial for mild traumatic
brain injury (mTBI), which is also associated with changes in brain energy needs. The effects of creatine supplementation on brain creatine levels, cognitive processing, and mTBI
have been previously reviewed [3,17,18]. As this is a growing field, the purpose of this
short review is to provide an update regarding the effects of creatine supplementation on
brain health in humans beyond what is discussed in Dolan et al. [3].
2. The Effects of Creatine Supplementation on Brain Creatine Levels
While muscle exclusively relies on dietary ingestion and endogenous synthesis from
the liver, kidneys, and pancreas [19], the brain can synthesize creatine. The enzymatic apparatus necessary for endogenous creatine synthesis is found in the nervous system and
creatine transporters are found at the blood–brain barrier, neurons and oligodendrocytes
cells, indicating that brain creatine may not solely dependent on endogenous production
from other organs or dietary sources [20]. Furthermore, brain creatine seems not to be
influenced by habitual dietary intake from food, as similar brain PCr is found between
vegetarians and omnivores [21]. Still, if the intracerebral synthesis is limited due to inherited disorders of creatine-catalyzing enzyme(s) machinery, dietary provision of the compound can positively affect brain creatine concentrations [22]. Figure 1 illustrates endogenous creatine synthesis in the brain and its transport across the blood–brain barrier.
Figure 1. Dietary creatine is transported through the blood–brain barrier via a creatine transporter. Astrocytes cells can
also endogenously produce creatine, which is taken up by the neurons expressing the creatine transporter.
Brain creatine content has been suggested to be affected by other factors, such as
aging [23]; however, comparable levels of brain PCr have also been found between apparently healthy elderly and young individuals [24]. Other factors related to aging that
may influence brain creatine concentrations include reduced brain and/or physical activity,
depression, schizophrenia, and panic disorder. The overlap between these factors may be
misleading as to what might be identified as an age-related decline (reviewed in Rawson
and Venezia [25]).
While consistent information is available on supplementation protocols aimed at
increasing muscle creatine content [26], much less is known regarding the optimal supplementation strategy to increase brain creatine levels. A large heterogeneity in respect
to brain creatine assessment technique (i.e., total brain creatine as assessed by H1
-NMR
vs. brain PCr as assessed by P31-NMR), supplement dose and duration (range 2 to 20 g/d
for 1 to 8 weeks), and population characteristics (including habitual dietary creatine intake, health status, etc.) hampers direct comparison between the few studies on the topic.
Further confusion is introduced by the fact that creatine content may differ regionally
within the brain [25,27]. Nevertheless, collectively, the available literature suggests possible increases in both creatine and PCr in the brain following supplementation, though
smaller than that seen in muscle (~half the increase) [3]. As reviewed in detail by Dolan
et al. [3], there are currently 12 studies of the effects of creatine supplementation on brain
creatine or PCr concentrations. Nine of these studies showed a significant increase in
brain creatine, averaging about 5 to 10%, which is less than the increase in muscle creatine
or PCr resulting from similar supplementation protocols. Some of these studies focused
on patient populations who have altered brain energetics, including females with major
depressive disorder, depression and amphetamine use, and selective serotonin uptake
inhibitor resistant depression. Other groups investigated the effects of creatine ingestion
on brain creatine levels in apparently healthy individuals. There is no clear indication why
a small number of studies were ineffective at increasing brain creatine despite using similar
supplementation protocols, but differences in baseline brain creatine levels, brain creatine
assessment, population characteristics, and dosing strategies likely play a role.
The explanation for these differences in creatine uptake between muscle and brain
remains speculative. As discussed, brain creatine content may rely less on exogenous
creatine than muscle [20,21,24,28], which could theoretically involve a down-regulated
response in brain creatine synthesis upon supplementation. Alternative to this hypothesis
is the demonstration that the brain lacks the expression of creatine transporter in the
astrocytes involved in the blood–brain barrier, thus implying a limited permeability of
the brain to the circulating creatine [29], which is in line with the lack of increase in
brain creatine following supplementation reported by some studies [24,28,30]. It is also
plausible to speculate that if the brain is, in fact, resistant to exogenous creatine, a highdose, long duration protocol would be needed, such as those used in the study by Dechent
et al. [27] (i.e., 20 g/day for 4 weeks). The need for a higher supplementation dose in
order to increase brain creatine level, as compared to the supplementation dose required
for muscle, is further corroborated by data available from the only study assessing both
muscle and brain creatine levels in response to supplementation, with increases found in
the former, but not the latter [24]. Of interest, supplementing guanidinoacetic acid (GAA),
a creatine precursor, was found superior to an equimolar dose of creatine in increasing
brain creatine content [31]. While creatine is mainly transported via a specific transporter
(SLC6A8 or CT1; also used for GAA transport), dietary GAA could be imported to the
brain through additional delivery transporters and routes (including SLC6A6, GAT2, and
passive diffusion) [32] and become readily available for methylation to creatine. Although
preliminary, these data are of relevance considering the inherent capacity of the brain to
synthesize creatine and its theoretical impaired ability to transport creatine through the
blood–brain barrier, thus warranting further research on alternative strategies to increase
brain creatine.
Nutrients 2021, 13, 586 4 of 10
3. Creatine Supplementation and Cognition
The interest in the effects of creatine supplementation on cognition is not new. Despite
the number of positive studies available on the subject (Summarized in Table 1), differences
between investigations including study populations, cognitive function testing, and supplementation dosing and duration precludes direct comparison; however, some conclusions
can be made. Although controversial [28,33,34], creatine supplementation may positively
influence some aspects of cognition in different experimental paradigms [10,35–40]. Importantly, its effects seem more pronounced in stressful conditions such as hypoxia [8]
and sleep deprivation combined with exercise [10,37,38]. Despite the suggestion that more
complex or demanding cognitive processes are more prone to respond to supplementation
(as they are more energy demanding), inconsistencies regarding cognitive test response to
supplementation hampers further conclusions [37,38]), which may be attributed to differences in experimental design such as the sleep deprivation period and exercise intensity
employed between studies.
Table 1. Effects of creatine supplementation on cognitive performance.
Population Creatine Supplementation
Protocol
Cognitive Tests (CT)
Outcomes (O) Reference
Healthy older women 20 g/day + 5 g/day for
24 weeks
CT: Mini-mental state examination, stroop, trail making,
digit span, delay recall test and the short version of the
geriatric depression scale
O: No change
Alves et al. (2013)
[33]
Semiprofessional,
non-vegetarian, male
mountain bikers
20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Simple and choice reaction time, differentiation task
test, Eiksen flanker test and Corsi block test
O: Creatine increased performance in choice reaction time,
Eiksen flanker test and Corsi block test.
Borchio et al. (2020)
[41]
Healthy young women
(vegetarian and
meat-eaters)
20 g/day for 5 days
CT: Word recall, simple and choice reaction time, rapid
visual information processing and controlled oral word
association test
O: Word recall test performance was reduced in meat-eater
after creatine supplementation (within-group comparison).
Post supplementation performance was higher in
vegetarians than in meat-eaters.
Benton and Donohoe
(2011) [15]
Professional male rugby
players who were
sleep-deprived (3–5 h)
0.05 or 0.1 g/kg/bw for
1 day
CT: Rugby passing skill test
O: Sleep deprivation reduced passing accuracy and
creatine reversed this effect (trend for greater effect with
larger dose).
Cook et al. (2011) [42]
Healthy young adults 20 g/day for 5 days +
5 g/day for 2 days
CT: Backward digit span test and ravens advanced
progressive matrices.
O: Backward digit span performance was increased after
creatine.
Hammett et al. (2010)
[35]
Healthy young men and
women
5 g/day for 15 days
CT: Memory scanning, number-pair matching, sustained
attention, arrow flankers and IQ test
O: Aspect of improvement was reported in all the cognitive
tests performed in the creatine group.
Ling et al. (2009) [36]
Healthy young men and
women who were
sleep-deprived (24 h)
20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Random number generation, forward and backward
recall, visual reaction time, static balance and mood state
O: Performance reduction was attenuated in the creatine
group for random movement generation, choice reaction
time, balance and mood.
McMorris et al. (2006)
[38]
Healthy elderly men and
women
20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Random number generation, forward and backward
recall and long-term memory tests
O: Forward number recall, forward and backward spatial
recall and long-term memory performance were enhanced
after creatine supplementation.
McMorris et al.
(2007a) [16]
Healthy young men who
were sleep-deprived (36 h) 20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Random number generation, short-term number recall,
visual reaction time, cognitive effort, dynamic balance test
and mood state
O: Performance on the random number generation test was
improved following creatine.
McMorris et al.
(2007b) [37]
Nutrients 2021, 13, 586 5 of 10
Table 1. Cont.
Population Creatine Supplementation
Protocol
Cognitive Tests (CT)
Outcomes (O) Reference
Healthy male and female
children 0.3 g/kg/day for 7 days
CT: Stroop, Rey auditory verbal learning test, Raven
progressive matrices and trail making test
O: No change
Merege-Filho et al.
(2017) [28]
Vegan and vegetarian
healthy male and female
young adults
5 g/day for 6 weeks
CT: Ravens advanced progressive matrices and Wechsler
auditory backward digit span task
O: Creatine improved performance on the Raven’s test and
the backward digit span task.
Rae and Broer (2015)
[17]
Healthy male and female
young adults 0.03 g/kg/day for 6 weeks CT: Automated neuropsychological assessment metrics
O: No change
Rawson et al. (2008)
[34]
Male and female
institutionalized older
adults (with full physical
and mental capacities
preserved)
5 g/day for 16 weeks
CT: Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) questionnaire
O: Creatine (plus resistance training) improved MoCA
scores.
Smolarek et al. (2020)
[43]
Healthy male and female
young adults exposed to
experimental hypoxia
20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Neuropsychological test comprising verbal and visual
memory, finger tapping, symbol digit coding stroop test,
test of shifting attention, continuous performance test,
alertness and peripheral and corticomotor excitability
O: Creatine supplementation offset hypoxia-induced
decrements in a number of cognitive tests.
Turner et al. (2015)
[10]
Healthy male and female
young adults exposed to
mental fatigue (90 min
Stoop task)
20 g/day for 7 days
CT: Psychomotor performance (visuomotor task with
Fitlight-hardware and software), strength endurance task,
Flanker test, heart rate, blood glucose, success motivation
and intrinsic motivation, mood, session ratings of
perceived exertion and mental fatigue
O: Accuracy throughout the 90 min Stroop task and
strength endurance (in the non-dominant hand) were
improved with creatine. No other effects of creatine
supplementation were observed.
Van Cutsem et al.
(2020) [44]
Healthy male and female
young adults 8 g/day for 5 days
CT: Serial calculation task (Uchida-Kraeplin)
O: Both groups increased mean performance. Mental
fatigue, assessed during the second half of the test, was
increased in the creatine group only.
Watanabe et al. (2002)
[40]
In elderly individuals, specifically, literature is conflicting on the effects of creatine
supplementation on cognitive performance. While McMorris et al. [16] showed improved
cognitive performance, Alves et al. [33] found creatine (alone or associated with exercise
training) ineffective. Both studies are limited by the lack of brain creatine concentration
assessments, casting doubt on whether aging-related reduction in cognitive processing
may arise from the presence of, for instance, neurodegenerative diseases or whether the
supplementation protocol employed (designed for increasing muscle creatine content) may
have been insufficient to significantly increase brain PCr. Recently, Smolarek et al. [43]
found increased cognitive performance (and handgrip strength) after a 16 week intervention combining resistance training and creatine supplementation (5 g/day) in a pilot study
including older adults. The results are, however, limited by the absence of an exercising
control group and inconsistent cognitive performance in the control group across time,
thus hampering further conclusion on the effects of supplementation alone.
It has been contended that vegetarians may differentially respond to creatine supplementation when compared to meat-eaters. In this respect, cognitive function has been
shown to be improved in vegetarians after creatine supplementation [39]. Another study
found greater effects on memory in vegetarians as compared to omnivores following creatine supplementation [15]. Importantly, the lack of a control group (meat-eaters) and the
fact that between-group differences were due to decreased performance in the omnivores,
rather than an improvement in the vegetarians, limits the conclusions of this study. Additionally, comparable brain creatine concentrations have been shown between meat-eaters
and vegetarians [21], which undermines the theory that vegetarians should respond better
Nutrients 2021, 13, 586 6 of 10
than meat-eaters due to lower pre-supplementation brain creatine. More research should be
conducted on the differential responses to creatine supplementation between vegetarians
and omnivores.
Improvements in cognitive processing capability is also of interest to athletes. Several
sports include motor control, decision making, coordination, reaction time, and other
cognitive tasks as key aspects of performance, which may be affected by mental fatigue [45].
In this respect, creatine may play an ergogenic role, as, theoretically, it may mitigate
mental fatigue, thus favouring performance. Indeed, creatine has been shown effective in
attenuating the effects of sleep deprivation on throwing accuracy in rugby players [42],
while no effect was observed on passing accuracy in non-stressed soccer players [46,47].
Brain creatine content was not assessed in these studies, raising uncertainty as to whether
the results observed result from changes in brain creatine. Nonetheless, the discrepancy in
the results may, at least partially, relate to the suggestion that creatine supplementation is
most effective under stressed cognitive processes conditions such as sleep deprivation.
More recently, two studies revisited the subject, with interesting results. Borchio
et al. [41] found improved performance in selected indexes of cognitive function after
a time-trial track test in semi-professional mountain bikers supplemented with creatine.
Interestingly, no prior cognitive deficit-inducing condition, such as sleep deprivation, was
imposed, suggesting that creatine could potentially attenuate mental fatigue even in nonstressed situations. Van Cutsem et al. [44] studied the effects of creatine supplementation
on mental fatigue and its negative effects on psychomotor skills in a non-athlete population
and found that creatine was able to improve Stroop accuracy during a 90 min Stroop
task and to increase strength endurance (assessed by a handgrip strength test) pre-to-post
Stroop task. Importantly, no effects of supplementation were observed on the mentalfatigue-induced impairments in psychomotor and cognitive performance. Collectively,
although these results suggest a potential role of creatine on mental fatigue, whether and
to what extent this could affect specific sports performance remains to be elucidated.
4. Creatine Supplementation and Brain Injury, Concussion, and Hypoxia
One of the characteristics of traumatic brain injury is the alteration of ATP demand due
to reduced blood flow and hypoxia [48]. Importantly, brain creatine is reduced following a
mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) [49], making creatine supplementation, and subsequent
increase in brain creatine, a potentially valuable strategy to reduce severity of, or enhance
recovery from, mTBI or concussion by offsetting negative changes in energy status. The
duration of the dysregulation in brain energy metabolism is not clearly defined, but could
remain for weeks if not years. Alosco et al. [50] reported on retired players from the National
Football League (aged 40 to 69) who had experienced repetitive head impacts during
their career and many years later had complaints of cognitive and/or behavioral/mood
symptoms. In this cohort, there was a relationship between greater exposure to repetitive
head impacts and decreased brain creatine in the parietal white matter. This indicates that
there could be later-life derangements in brain energy metabolism subsequent to mTBI, and
lends support to the concept that creatine supplementation could be valuable in enhancing
recovery from mTBI, even years after the injury. In addition to its potential role in aiding
the cellular energy crisis induced by injury, creatine may lessen other features of mTBI,
such as membrane disruption leading to calcium influx, nerve damage, mitochondrial
dysfunction, oxidative stress, and inflammation (reviewed in [48,51]).
In an experimental model mimicking the effects of mTBI, Turner et al. [10] found
that supplementation was able to increase brain creatine and cognitive processing during
oxygen deprivation. Animal models have also been employed to investigate the effects of
creatine supplementation on traumatic brain injury. Sullivan et al. [52] found significant
reduction in brain damage following traumatic brain injury in both mice (36%) and rats
(50%). These large effects are compelling, but as humans only increase brain creatine
about 10% in response to supplementation and some animals increase brain creatine 30
to 50%, it is difficult to generalize these data to the general population or athletes [53].
Nutrients 2021, 13, 586 7 of 10
Despite its potential, experimental data in humans are still scarce; however, results from
the few studies available are promising. Creatine supplementation has been shown able to
improve cognition, communication, self-care, personality, and behavior, and reductions in
headaches, dizziness, and fatigue in children with mTBI [54,55].
Collectively, despite limited data, creatine supplementation seems potentially beneficial in reducing severity of or enhancing recovery from mTBI, warranting further studies
on its role not only as a post-injury therapy but also as a neuroprotective agent in populations at high risk of mTBI. As has been described elsewhere, creatine supplements have
documented muscular performance benefits, are inexpensive, widely available, and have
a strong safety profile [26,56–63]. Encouraging supplementation to reduce damage from
or enhance recovery from mTBI based primarily on animal and theoretical data in lieu
of clinical trials would ordinarily be considered premature. However, in this instance,
given the devastating effects of mTBI, combined with the large body of safety and efficacy
creatine supplementation data, encouraging supplementation for populations who are at
high risk for mTBI might be considered more prudent.
No comments:
Post a Comment