You'll have to demand your doctor get these protocols. S/he has only had 7 years to get them so ask why they are so fucking incompetent. And the earliest research referred to is back to 2000, so 20 years of incompetency, how fucking long will you allow that to continue? It is your responsibility to get competence in your stroke hospital, that leadership(board of directors) is completely failing.
mental practice (7 posts to July 2015)
motor imagery (48 posts to January 2013)
motor learning (9 posts to June 2015)
Towards the integration of mental practice in rehabilitation programs. A critical review
- 1Département de Réadaptation, Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada
- 2Centre Interdisciplinaire de Recherche en Réadaptation et Intégration Sociale, Québec, QC, Canada
- 3École de Psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada
- 4Centre de Recherche de l'Institut Universitaire en Santé Mentale de Québec, Québec, QC, Canada
Introduction
The ever-increasing number of publications attests to
clinician expectations of mental practice (MP) through motor imagery
(MI) as a means of promoting the recovery of motor function (for a
review see Malouin and Richards, 2013).
MP not only provides a unique opportunity to increase the number of
repetitions in a safe and autonomous manner without undue physical
fatigue, but it also allows the mental rehearsal of motor tasks when and
where the patient wants to, or is able to, practice. Furthermore, MP
enables the rehearsal of more demanding or complex motor tasks (e.g.,
walking, writing) when physical practice is impossible or too difficult.
Yet, despite these obvious advantages, MP is a complex mental process
that is not readily amenable to be integrated into clinical practice. To
date, in most published studies, MP has been used within constrained
research environments to meet the requirements associated with research
methodology.
As highlighted by several review papers concerning the use of MP in rehabilitation, (van Leeuwen and Inglis, 1998; Jackson et al., 2001; Braun et al., 2006; Dickstein and Deutsch, 2007; Zimmermann-Schlatter et al., 2008; Dijkerman et al., 2010; Malouin and Richards, 2010, 2013)
there are marked differences in designs, research protocols, training
regimens and outcome measures among the growing number of studies.
Despite this heterogeneity, positive effects of MP on motor function
have been generally reported. However, Braun et al. (2006),
in a systematic review of five selected randomized controlled trials
(RCT), stated that although there was some evidence that MP as an
adjunct therapeutic intervention had beneficial effects on arm function,
they were not able to draw definite conclusions(Well then, write up a provisional protocol, waiting for perfection is stupid.) and stated that further
research with a clear definition of the content of the MP and
standardized outcome measures were needed. In a more recent review that
included six studies, Barclay-Goddard et al. (2011)
also concluded that the combination of MP with other treatments
appeared to be more effective than other treatments alone to improve
upper extremity function. Based on their assessment with the PEDro
scale, the quality of the evidence was moderate. Likewise, in their
systematic review of 15 studies, Nilsen et al. (2010)
attested that when MP was added to physical practice (PP), it was an
effective intervention. Nevertheless, they also mentioned that further
research was needed to identify those patients most likely to benefit
from training, the optimal dose, and the most effective protocols.
These reviews, however, did not include the findings originating from recent multicenter RCTs (Bovend'Eerdt et al., 2010; Ietswaart et al., 2011; Braun et al., 2012; Timmermans et al., 2013)
in subacute patients that have attempted to integrate MI training in
regular rehabilitation programs. Not only did the addition of MP to
conventional training on all tasks fail to yield better functional
outcomes than conventional training, but the low compliance of
therapists (Bovend'Eerdt et al., 2010; Braun et al., 2010, 2012) and realities related to patients such as advanced age of those in nursing homes (Braun et al., 2012)
point to some of the difficulties encountered when attempting to
introduce MP into regular clinical practice. The findings of two recent
RCTs, (Ietswaart et al., 2011; Timmermans et al., 2013)
did not confirm the additional benefits of including MI training in the
rehabilitation program aimed at improving upper limb function. Despite
meticulously designed MI training that included a variety of approaches
(action observation through mirror therapy, implicit imagery, and
self-practice), patients with subacute stroke did not show additional
gains in the performance of activities of daily living (ADL) (Ietswaart et al., 2011).
Altogether, these latest findings reflect the complexity of integrating
MP into regular rehabilitation programs. Thus, this review scrutinizes
the current application of MP, and from this analysis proposes a
framework for its integration into usual rehabilitation programs.
Rationale Underlying MI Training
With the turn of the twenty-first century, we have
witnessed the emergence of clinical studies designed to investigate the
effects of MP on the relearning of motor skills in persons with stroke.
The rationale for using MI training to promote the relearning of motor
function arises from research on the functional correlates that MI
shares with the execution of physical movements. It is now widely
recognized that the duration of mentally simulated actions usually
correlates with the duration of real movements (temporal coupling), that
the simulation of movements evokes similar autonomic responses and that
the imagination of an action or its physical execution engage largely
similar neural networks (Decety and Boisson, 1990; Decety et al., 1991; Decety and Jeannerod, 1995; Wuyam et al., 1995; Decety, 1996; Decety and Grèzes, 1999; Lafleur et al., 2002; Malouin et al., 2003; Fusi et al., 2005; Munzert and Zentgraf, 2009; Hétu et al., 2013).
These similarities led to the notion of functional equivalence. Thus,
real and covert movements during MI obey similar principles and share
similar neural mechanisms, likely explaining the beneficial effects of
MP on motor performance (Jeannerod, 1995).
No comments:
Post a Comment