Use the labels in the right column to find what you want. Or you can go thru them one by one, there are only 29,286 posts. Searching is done in the search box in upper left corner. I blog on anything to do with stroke. DO NOT DO ANYTHING SUGGESTED HERE AS I AM NOT MEDICALLY TRAINED, YOUR DOCTOR IS, LISTEN TO THEM. BUT I BET THEY DON'T KNOW HOW TO GET YOU 100% RECOVERED. I DON'T EITHER BUT HAVE PLENTY OF QUESTIONS FOR YOUR DOCTOR TO ANSWER.
Changing stroke rehab and research worldwide now.Time is Brain!trillions and trillions of neuronsthatDIEeach day because there areNOeffective hyperacute therapies besides tPA(only 12% effective). I have 523 posts on hyperacute therapy, enough for researchers to spend decades proving them out. These are my personal ideas and blog on stroke rehabilitation and stroke research. Do not attempt any of these without checking with your medical provider. Unless you join me in agitating, when you need these therapies they won't be there.
What this blog is for:
My blog is not to help survivors recover, it is to have the 10 million yearly stroke survivors light fires underneath their doctors, stroke hospitals and stroke researchers to get stroke solved. 100% recovery. The stroke medical world is completely failing at that goal, they don't even have it as a goal. Shortly after getting out of the hospital and getting NO information on the process or protocols of stroke rehabilitation and recovery I started searching on the internet and found that no other survivor received useful information. This is an attempt to cover all stroke rehabilitation information that should be readily available to survivors so they can talk with informed knowledge to their medical staff. It lays out what needs to be done to get stroke survivors closer to 100% recovery. It's quite disgusting that this information is not available from every stroke association and doctors group.
Tuesday, February 28, 2017
How Brain Scientists Forgot That Brains Have Owners
It’s a good time to be interested in the brain. Neuroscientists can now turn neurons on or off with just a flash of light, allowing them to manipulate the behavior of animals with exceptional precision. They can turn brains transparent and seed them with glowing molecules to divine their structure. They can record the activity of huge numbers of neurons at once. And those are just the tools that currently exist. In 2013, Barack Obama launched the Brain Research through Advancing Innovative Neurotechnologies (BRAIN) Initiative—a $115 million plan to develop even better technologies for understanding the enigmatic gray blobs that sit inside our skulls. John Krakaeur,
a neuroscientist at Johns Hopkins Hospital, has been asked to BRAIN
Initiative meetings before, and describes it like “Maleficent being
invited to Sleeping Beauty’s birthday.” That’s because he and four
like-minded friends have become increasingly disenchanted by their
colleagues’ obsession with their toys. And in a new paper
that’s part philosophical treatise and part shot across the bow, they
argue that this technological fetish is leading the field astray.
“People think technology + big data + machine learning = science,” says
Krakauer. “And it’s not.” He and his fellow curmudgeons argue that brains are special because of the behavior
they create—everything from a predator’s pounce to a baby’s cry. But
the study of such behavior is being de-prioritized, or studied “almost
as an afterthought.” Instead, neuroscientists have been focusing on
using their new tools to study individual neurons, or networks of
neurons. According to Krakauer, the unspoken assumption is that if we
collect enough data about the parts, the workings of the whole will
become clear. If we fully understand the molecules that dance across a
synapse, or the electrical pulses that zoom along a neuron, or the web
of connections formed by many neurons, we will eventually solve the
mysteries of learning, memory, emotion, and more. “The fallacy is that
more of the same kind of work in the infinitely postponed future will
transform into knowing why that mother’s crying or why I’m feeling this
way,” says Krakauer. And, as he and his colleagues argue, it will not.
That’s because behavior is an emergent
property—it arises from large groups of neurons working together, and
isn’t apparent from studying any single one. You can draw parallels with
the flocking of birds. Biologists have long wondered how they manage to
wheel about the skies in perfect coordination, as if they were a single
entity. In the 1980s, computer scientists showed that this can happen
if each bird obeys a few simple rules, which dictate their distance and
alignment relative to their peers. From these simple individual rules,
collective complexity emerges.But
you would never have been able to predict the latter from the former.
No matter how thoroughly you understood the physics of feathers, you
could never have predicted a murmuration of starlings
without first seeing it happen. So it is with the brain. As British
neuroscientist David Marr wrote in 1982, “trying to understand
perception by understanding neurons is like trying to understand a
bird’s flight by studying only feathers. It just cannot be done.” A landmark study, published last year, beautifully illustrated his point using, of all things, retro video games. Eric Jonas and Konrad Kording examined the MOS 6502 microchip, which ran classics like Donkey Kong and Space Invaders,
in the style of neuroscientists. Using the approaches that are common
to brain science, they wondered if they could rediscover what they
already knew about the chip—how its transistors and logic gates process
information, and how they run simple games. And they utterly failed.
“What
we extracted was so incredibly superficial,” Jonas told me last year.
And “in the real world, this would be a millions-of-dollars data set.”
If the kind of neuroscience that has come to dominate the field couldn’t
explain the workings of a simple, dated microchip, how could it hope to
explain the brain—reputedly the most complex object in the universe?
No comments:
Post a Comment