http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00098/full
- 1Center for Vital Longevity, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
- 2Department of Psychology, The University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, USA
Introduction
Everyday conversation frequently touches upon the
busyness of daily schedules. People discuss their packed to-do lists and
make inferences about the impact of their busy lifestyle on their
health and mental function. Often busyness carries a negative
connotation, as people tend to complain about their hectic schedules,
yet, little scientific work has been done to empirically investigate the
construct of busyness and its associations. To fill this gap, the
present study: (1) assesses whether busier people tend to have better or
worse cognitive performance; and (2) tests whether this relationship
with cognition varies with age.
Busyness has been defined as the subjective evaluation
of one’s ongoing activity patterns, including reflections about the
quantity of one’s unscheduled time and comparisons to what is expected
or standard (see Gershuny, 2005; Levine, 2005). Martin and Park (2003)
developed a self-report assessment of day-to-day busyness in the Martin
and Park Environmental Demands Questionnaire (MPED). This questionnaire
yields two scale scores (i.e., Busyness and Routines), and the Busyness
scale asks individuals to reflect upon, for example, how frequently
they have too many tasks to complete or too little time in the day. The
Busyness measure had high internal consistency and external validity, as
it was related to medication adherence, employment status, and
household size. We utilize this measure of busyness in the present study
and examine its relationship to cognition.
Given the pervasive discussion of busyness in everyday
life, it is surprising that few studies have assessed busyness. We note
that, unlike engagement, which typically bears a positive connotation,
busyness carries a more negative undertone, and, at present, the
cognitive associations of a busy lifestyle are empirically unknown.
Related literature suggests that busyness either could be beneficial or
harmful to cognition. Busyness could be related to increased levels of
stress, which can have negative consequences on the brain and cognitive
function (i.e., allostatic load, see McEwen, 1998).
For instance, stress hormones have been shown to have negative neural
impacts, with different brain regions showing more vulnerabilities at
different points in the lifespan (for a review see Lupien et al., 2009).
Moreover, stress has been shown to narrow attention, impair working
memory (i.e., potentially by disrupting encoding and maintenance
processes), interfere with knowledge acquisition, and degrade
perceptual-motor performance (see Staal, 2004). High stress even increased the risk of death, although this was only true in people who viewed stress as harmful (Keller et al., 2012).
Thus, it is possible that individuals who are very busy could have
inferior cognitive function relative to their less busy counterparts.
Alternatively, more positively, busyness could be
related to increased effortful engagement at work, home, and in leisure
activities, which can have advantageous consequences on neural health
and cognition. Recently, several studies experimentally manipulated
lifestyle engagement levels and found benefits for intense, sustained
engagement. In the Synapse Project, productive engagement groups that
learned digital photography or quilting showed improvements in episodic
memory relative to receptive control groups that did little new learning
(Park et al., 2014), and this sustained new learning also resulted in more efficient neural processing (McDonough et al., 2015).
An iPad training project similarly found that prolonged engagement in
learning to use an iPad resulted in improvements in episodic memory and
processing speed (Chan et al., 2014).
Moreover, participants in the Experience Corps program improved their
memory and executive functioning after prolonged mentoring of elementary
school students (Carlson et al., 2008),
and participants engaging in the Senior Odyssey curriculum improved
their processing speed, inductive reasoning, and divergent thinking
after sustained mental engagement (Stine-Morrow et al., 2008). Finally, theater training has also been shown to improve memory and problem solving in older adults (Noice et al., 2004).
In addition to these experimental manipulations of
engagement, many correlational studies report benefits of high levels of
cognitive, social, and physical activities. Benefits include improved
cognition, delayed cognitive decline, increased longevity, and reduced
risk of various diseases, including dementias (e.g., see Christensen and Mackinnon, 1993; Glass et al., 1999; Singh-Manoux et al., 2003; Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2007; Wilson et al., 2007; Seeman et al., 2011; Small et al., 2012).
Furthermore, greater work complexity (i.e., greater opportunity to
perform higher cognitive operations; more independent work) has been
associated with better cognition and longevity (see Correa Ribeiro et al., 2013; Then et al., 2013; Andel et al., 2015; Massimo et al., 2015).
For instance, higher mental demand at work was related to better
cognition at baseline and a slower rate of cognitive decline over 8
years (Then et al., 2015).
Based on these experimental and correlational findings,
if busyness serves as a proxy for intense, sustained lifestyle
engagement, then we would predict that greater busyness would be
associated with better cognition. Moreover, because busyness has been
shown to differ between middle-aged adults and older adults (see Martin and Park, 2003),
we aimed to examine if the effects of busyness on cognition were
consistent across adults aged 50–89. It may be that the greatest effects
of busyness will be observed in older adults, who tend to have more
deficiencies in cognition compared to their younger counterparts (i.e., Park et al., 2002),
and thus, may be more sensitive to the effects of busyness. In line
with this hypothesis, some studies of activity levels and work
complexity have found larger effects in older adults than middle-aged
adults and young adults (e.g., Hultsch et al., 1993; Schooler et al., 1999).
On the other hand, busyness could be detrimental to cognition if it
heightens stress substantially, as prolonged stress is harmful to the
central nervous system (i.e., Lupien et al., 2009).
Overall, the goal of the current study is to examine the relationship
between busyness and cognition. We interpret our results in the context
of other relevant literature on busyness, engagement, and activity
levels and also discuss hypothetical mechanisms.
No comments:
Post a Comment