http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00213/full?
Maddalena Boccia1,2, Laura Piccardi2,3*, Pierluigi Cordellieri1, Cecilia Guariglia1,2 and Anna Maria Giannini1
- 1Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- 2Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia of Rome, Rome, Italy
- 3Department of Life, Health and Environmental Sciences, L’Aquila University, L’Aquila, Italy
Introduction
Traumatic events (including not only large-scale
disasters but also common day-to-day events, such as Motor vehicle
accidents (MVAs) are an important cause of psychological distress and
psychiatric disorders. Harvey and Bryant (1998) reports the presence of acute stress disorders (ASD) in 13% of MVA survivors, and according to Mayou et al. (1993)
one year after a MVA a quarter of those followed up showed psychiatric
disorders, with 11% affected by post-traumatic stress disorders (PTSD).
PTSD is a relatively common psychiatric disorder
occurring as a consequence of a major traumatic event. It is clinically
characterized by the following symptoms: involuntarily re-experiencing
phenomena (e.g., nightmares, flashbacks, intrusive images as well as
recurrent distressing thoughts of the event); avoidance of talking about
or being reminded of the traumatic event, negative alterations in
thoughts and mood, emotional numbing and hyperarousal symptoms (e.g.,
sleep disturbance, difficulty in concentrating, increased irritability
and hypervigilance) (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2004).
Different types of psychological therapies have been proposed in the treatment of PTSD, including exposure therapy (Creamer et al., 2004), cognitive therapy (Resick and Schnicke, 1992; Ehlers et al., 2005), psychodynamic psychotherapy (Brom et al., 1989) and eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR; Shapiro, 1989).
EMDR is currently an effective psychological treatment, recognized and
recommended as a firstline treatment for trauma in numerous
international guidelines (Bisson and Andrew, 2007). According to the review by Ponniah and Hollon (2009)
EMDR ameliorates PTSD symptoms significantly more than waiting list,
standard care, and pill placebo. They also reported that a number of
studies found that EMDR was superior to trauma-focused cognitive
behavioral therapies (CBT) on some measures of PTSD symptoms. However,
all of these studies had mixed trauma samples. Another data coming from
this review is that between 77 and 90% of EMDR patients no longer met
diagnostic criteria for PTSD at the end of treatment. Ponniah and Hollon
findings provide support for the use of EMDR for all patients with
PTSD.
EMDR is a supplementary trauma-focused therapy that
includes elements from other effective psychotherapies in a structured
protocol drawn from an information processing model of PTSD (Bisson et al., 2013).
It requires the individual suffering from PTSD to focus attention on a
traumatic memory whilst simultaneously visually tracking the therapist’s
finger as it moves across his/her visual field, and then to engage in a
restructuring of the memory (Shapiro, 1995).
Eye movements are the most common form of bilateral stimulation, but
stimulation might also be auditory (alternating tones) or sensory
(finger tapping). It acts by using dual attention tasks to help the
patient process the traumatic event while focusing on negative
trauma-related memories, emotions and thoughts during the performance of
a task that requires a bilateral stimulation (e.g., eye movements; hand
tapping; tones) until a growth in more positive trauma-related thoughts
(Jensen, 1994; Shepherd et al., 2000; Marcus et al., 2004).
Since its discovery, EMDR has been considered one of the
treatments of choice for PTSD, even though studies on its effectiveness
are often hindered by methodological problems (see for a critical
review Cahill et al., 1999),
and in the view of some authors “what is effective in EMDR (imaginal
exposure) is not new, and what is new (eye movements) is not effective” (McNally, 1999, p. 2). Although bilateral stimulation is discussed controversially (Cahill et al., 1999), growing evidence has demonstrated the effectiveness of EMDR in treating both PTSD in victims and mourning in survivors (Sprang, 2001; Solomon and Rando, 2007; see also meta-analysis studies: Bisson et al., 2013; Lee and Cuijpers, 2013; Watts et al., 2013). Lee and Cuijpers (2013)
performed a meta-analysis in which 15 clinical and 11 experimental
studies demonstrated different effects of bilateral stimulation through
eye movements compared with those produced by other exposure therapies.
Possible explanations for the effectiveness of alternating bilateral
stimulation are: stimulation acts specifically on disintegrated
information related to the traumatic event, or boosts the processing of
emotionally memories or, last but not least, may enhance emotional
processing in general (Sprang, 2001; Korn and Leeds, 2002). Herkt et al. (2014)
recently observed in healthy subjects without post-traumatic symptoms
increased activation in the right amygdala during alternating auditory
bilateral stimulation, as used in EMDR, while processing emotionally
negative stimuli. These authors suggested that the increase in limbic
processing along with decreased frontal activation is in line with
theoretical models (Shapiro, 1989, 2002) of how alternating bilateral stimulation might help with the therapeutic reintegration of information. Specifically, Shapiro (1989)
suggests two possible interpretations for the effects of alternating
bilateral stimulation: (i) it may boost the processing of any
emotionally laden material in general; or (ii) it may have a specific
effect just on disintegrated information related to the traumatic
episode. Clinicians also observe a decrease in vividness and arousal
related to trauma-associated stimuli after EMDR, and neuroimaging
studies show that after EMDR there is a decreased activation in limbic
areas and increased activation in prefrontal brain regions known to be
responsible for cognitive control after the completion of successful
treatments (Lansing et al., 2005; Pagani et al., 2007).
Clinical trials suggest that different traumatic events interact with
individual factors (such as personality, gender and genetic factors) and
lead to different physical and behavioral outcomes as well as a
different prevalence of PTSD (Ditlevsen and Elklit, 2012; Santiago et al., 2013; Husarewycz et al., 2014; Perrin et al., 2014).
Even if altered brain areas after PTSD are common and play
complementary roles in maintaining the PTSD symptomatology, such as fear
conditioning of trauma-related stimuli and failing to recall fear
extinction (Pitman et al., 2012).
However, specific network of areas could be observed due to specific
trauma. In details, PTSD after physical or sexual abuse modifies
specific brain structures including the middle and anterior cingulate
cortex (MCC; ACC), precuneus (pCU) and middle frontal gyrus (see Shin et al., 1999; Lanius et al., 2002, 2005).
These brain regions are involved in pain processing, fear, sadness and
proprioceptive information. Differently, in the PTSD after
combat-related trauma alterations have been found in a network of areas
including the bilateral insula, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), posterior
cingulate cortex (PCC), superior parietal lobe (SPL) and hippocampus (Pissiota et al., 2002; Britton et al., 2005; Geuze et al., 2007; Morey et al., 2008). Also these structures are known to be involved in emotional processing, especially of sadness (Vogt, 2005),
and in monitoring internal body states, but they are also involved in a
wide range of cognitive functions, including episodic memory, spatial
navigation, imagining and planning for the future (Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Vann et al., 2009; Boccia et al., 2014).
A specific network of areas is present also in PTSD after catastrophe
and includes the bilateral parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), right superior
temporal gyrus (STG) and superior frontal gyrus (SFG; Hou et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2009; Mazza et al., 2012). Specifically, the PHG has a crucial role in spatial navigation and in scene perception (Epstein and Morgan, 2012)
and it is reported only in this kind of trauma, likely due to the fact
that the natural disasters mostly involved the surrounding environment
and familiar places. Taking together these fMRI studies seem suggest
that PTSD due to different kind of trauma can be different from a
neurological and cognitive point of view. As a consequence also the
variability in the psychological therapies effectiveness could be
partially explained by the existence of different neural substrates
underpinning the main disorder.
In view of this evidence, the main aim of the present
study is to examine the extent to which neurobiological evidence
supports the specific treatment of PTSD after MVA with EMDR. To pursue
this aim we first reviewed previous neuroimaging studies about PTSD
after MVA and those about neural correlates of EMDR. We hypothesized
that by modulating the dysfunctional network of PTSD-MVA, EMDR may be
the treatment of choice for patients who develop PTSD after a MVA. To
test this hypothesis, we performed a meta-analysis of fMRI studies on
PTSD after MVA to assess neural network functional changes in people
suffering from PTSD following a MVA, using activation likelihood
estimation (ALE; Eickhoff et al., 2009).
Results have been discussed in light of current evidence about the
neural underpinnings of EMDR, which suggests a specific and
biologically-based approach to PTSD after MVA.
No comments:
Post a Comment