Way beyond my pay grade, ask your doctor for what this means in 8th grade language. And then ask for EXACT PROTOCOLS THAT PREVENT THESE PROBLEMS.
Defining the relationship between hypertension, cognitive decline, and dementia: a review
The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Curr Hypertens Rep
See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.
Abstract
Hypertension
is a highly prevalent condition which has been established as a risk
factor for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease. Although the
understanding of the relationship between cardiocirculatory dysfunction
and brain health has improved significantly over the last several
decades, it is still unclear whether hypertension constitutes a
potentially treatable risk factor for cognitive decline and dementia.
While it is clear that hypertension can affect brain structure and
function, recent findings suggest that the associations between blood
pressure and brain health are complex and, in many cases, dependent on
factors such as age, hypertension chronicity, and antihypertensive
medication use. Whereas large epidemiological studies have demonstrated a
consistent association between high midlife BP and late-life cognitive
decline and incident dementia, associations between late-life blood
pressure and cognition have been less consistent. Recent evidence
suggests that hypertension may promote alterations in brain structure
and function through a process of cerebral vessel remodeling, which can
lead to disruptions in cerebral autoregulation, reductions in cerebral
perfusion, and limit the brain’s ability to clear potentially harmful
proteins such as β-amyloid. The purpose of the current review is to
synthesize recent findings from epidemiological, neuroimaging,
physiological, genetic, and translational research to provide an
overview of what is currently known about the association between blood
pressure and cognitive function across the lifespan. In doing so, the
current review also discusses the results of recent randomized
controlled trials of antihypertensive therapy to reduce cognitive
decline, highlights several methodological limitations, and provides
recommendations for future clinical trial design.
Keywords: Hypertension, Hypotension, Blood Pressure, Cognition, Cognitive Impairment, Dementia
Introduction
Hypertension
is a highly prevalent condition, occurring in one-third of the world’s
adults and two-thirds of adults over age 65 [1,2]. Already an established risk factor for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease [3–6],
emerging evidence suggests that hypertension may also play an important
role in the development of cognitive decline, Alzheimer’s disease, and
vascular dementia [7–9].
Because hypertension is a modifiable risk factor, it represents a
potentially important mechanism through which the prevention or delay of
age-related cognitive disorders may be possible. For this reason,
understanding hypertension’s role in the development and progression of
age-related cognitive decline and dementia has been a research priority
over the last two decades. Although a great deal has been learned from
epidemiological studies, there is still little consensus about the
effectiveness of treating hypertension to prevent or slow cognitive
decline. What is clear, however, is that the connection between blood
pressure (BP) and cognitive function is biologically complex and still
not fully understood.
The goal of this
review is to provide an overview of the research that has contributed to
the understanding of the connection between BP and cognitive function,
paying particular attention to recent findings. In doing so, this review
will first provide an overview of what is known about the connection
between hypertension, cognitive function, Alzheimer’s disease, and
vascular dementia. Second, the neurobiological changes associated with
hypertension will be described, and the research that demonstrates how
these biological processes influence neuronal function will be
highlighted. Lastly, the findings from clinical trials designed to
assess the effectiveness of antihypertensive agents for the prevention
or delay of cognitive decline will be summarized. Methodological
considerations and specific recommendations for future research will
also be discussed. Although this review focuses on the topic of
hypertension and cognitive function, the link between low BP and
cognition will also be discussed.
Hypertension and cognitive function
Cross-sectional and longitudinal observational studies
Over
the last several decades the link between hypertension and cognitive
function has been examined across many age groups. Although much of this
research has focused on understanding the relationship between BP and
cognition in older adults, the group most likely to experience cognitive
decline, studies which assess BP starting in middle-age and follow
participants forward until they reach older ages have also been
especially informative. Multiple epidemiological studies have
demonstrated that elevated BP in the 4th and 5th, decade of life, particularly untreated hypertension, increases the risk for cognitive impairment 20–30 years later (see Table 1) [10–12].
These findings have been further supported by longitudinal studies
which show that high midlife BP is associated with increased cognitive
decline over time [13–15].
Because confounding variables, such as education and socioeconomic
status, are less likely to affect cognitive change (compared to baseline
cognitive abilities) [16],
studies which show an increased rate of cognitive decline over time
among hypertensive adults provide especially strong evidence for the
deleterious effects of high BP. As will be discussed below, several
studies have also identified hypertension duration and the trajectory of
BP levels over time as important determinants of cognitive function
later in life [17,18].
Table 1.
Study | Study Design Duration n | Age at BP Assessment | BP Measure | Age at Cognitive Assessment | Cognitive Domains Assessed | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NHANES III, USA [40] | Cross-sectional n = 5,077 | 6-16 | SBP, DBP | 6-16 | Arithmetic Reading Visuospatial Working memory | Elevated SBP, but not DBP, was associated with poorer working memory. |
Normative Aging Study, USA [18] | Cross-temporal a 29 years n = 758 | 37 (5) (BP measured every 3-5 years for up to 44 years) | HTN | 66 (5) | Multi-domain composite score | HTN at any point during follow-up and greater duration since onset of HTN were associated with lower cognitive functioning later in life, independent of age at onset. |
NHLBI Twin Study, USA [194] | Cross-temporal a 25 years Longitudinal 10 years n = 392 | 43-53 | SBP, DBP | T1: 59-69 T2: 68-79 | MMSE Processing speed Verbal fluency Visual memory | Elevated SBP at baseline was associated with declines in processing speed over a period of 10 years. |
Honolulu-Asia Aging Study, USA [12] | Cross-temporal a 25 years n = 3,735 | 53 (5) | SBP, DBP | 78 (5) | Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI) | Elevated SBP at baseline was associated with poorer cognitive functioning in late-life. |
Xi’an, China [30] | Cross-sectional n = 1,799 | 40-85 | SBP, DBP, MAP | 40-85 | MMSE | Elevated SBP, DBP, and MAP were associated with cognitive impairment among 40-60 year-olds, but this was not the case for older participants. |
Whitehall II, UK [195] | Cross-temporal a 12 years n = 5,838 | 44 (6) | SBP, DBP | 56 | Memory Reasoning Verbal fluency Vocabulary | Elevated SBP at baseline was associated with poorer baseline memory and reduced verbal fluency at follow-up, especially among women. |
REGARDS, USA [196] | Cross-temporal a 3 years n = 17,630 | 64 (9) | HTN | 67 | Multi-domain composite score | HTN was not associated with the development of cognitive impairment over a span of 40 months. |
Framingham Heart Study, USA [197] | Cross-temporal a 12 years n = 1,814 | 40-69 | HTN | T1: 52 (8) T2: 64 | Executive function Memory Visuospatial | HTN was associated with worse performance on measures of executive functioning and visual memory. |
NHANES III, USA [198] | Cross-sectional n = 3,270 | 30-59 | HTN | 30-59 | Processing speed Reaction time Working memory | HTN and DM, but not HTN alone, was associated with worse reaction time, processing speed, and working memory. |
EVA Study Group, France [199] | Longitudinal 4 years n = 1,373 | 59-71 | HTN | T1: 59-71 T2: 63-75 | MMSE | Baseline HTN was associated greater MMSE declines over 4 years. This relationship was stronger among participants who were untreated for hypertension. |
ARIC, USA [200] | Longitudinal 6 years n = 10,963 | 47-70 | HTN | T1: 47-70 T2: 53-76 | Memory Processing speed Verbal fluency | Baseline HTN was associated with greater decline in processing speed over a 6-year period. |
ARIC, USA [14] | Longitudinal 14 years n = 12,702 | 59 (4) | HTN | T1: 59 (4) T2: 62 T3: 65 T4: 73 | Memory Processing speed Verbal fluency | Baseline HTN was associated with greater declines in verbal fluency over a 14-year period. |
ARIC, USA [13] | Longitudinal 20 years n = 13,476 | 45-64 | Pre-HTN, HTN | T1: 48-67 T2: 54-73 T3: 70-89 | Processing speed Verbal fluency Verbal memory | Baseline HTN was associated with greater declines in processing speed, verbal fluency, and a global composite score of cognitive functioning over a 20-year period. |
Western Collaborative Group Study, USA [17] | Cross-temporal a 25-30 years n = 717 | 39-59 (BP measured approximately 10 times over 30 years) | SBP | 75 (4) | Executive function Memory Processing speed | Compared to participants who maintained a normal SBP over 30 years, participants who had persistently high SBP had poorer verbal memory in late-life. Participants who displayed a significant decrease in SBP over 30 years performed more poorly on measure of processing speed. |
Male Cohort in Uppsala, Sweden [11] | Cross-temporal a 20 years n = 999 | 50 | SBP, DBP | 72 | Multi-domain composite score | Elevated DBP at baseline was associated with reduced cognitive functioning in late-life. This relationship was stronger among participants who were untreated for hypertension. |
Male Cohort in Uppsala, Sweden [10] | Cross-temporal a 20 years n = 502 | 50 | SBP, DBP | 72 (1) | Memory Processing speed Verbal fluency Visuospatial Working memory | Elevated DBP at baseline was associated with poorer performance on measures of working memory, processing speed, and verbal fluency in late-life. Participants with DBP ≤ 70mg Hg at baseline demonstrated highest levels cognitive functioning in late-life. |
Framingham, USA [201] | Cross-temporal a 28 years n = 1,993 | 27-87 (BP measured biennially over 28 years) | HTN, SBP, DBP | 55-89 | Attention Language Memory Visuospatial | Among participants untreated for hypertension, the proportion of visits during which HTN was present and the average SBP and DBP were inversely associated with cognitive functioning. |
ARIC, USA [202] | Cross-sectional n = 13,840 | 45-69 | HTN | 45-69 | Processing speed Verbal fluency Verbal memory | HTN among women, but not men, was associated with poorer performance on all cognitive measures. |
ARIC
= Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities; BP = blood pressure; DBP =
diastolic blood pressure; EVA = Epidemiology of Vascular Aging; HTN =
hypertension; MAP = mean arterial pressure; MMSE = Mini Mental Status
Exam; NHANES III = National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey;
NHLBI = National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute; REGARDS = Reasons for
Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke; SBP = systolic blood
pressure
aCross temporal: a study
design in which the exposure variable (e.g., hypertension) and the
outcome variable (e.g., cognition) are measured distict time points.
Hypertension in the 6th and 7th decade has been associated with poorer overall cognitive function and cognitive decline (see Table 2) [19–23].
Hypertension among individuals in their 70s has also been identified as
a risk factor for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) – a state of subtle
cognitive decline that is believed to precede the onset of dementia [24,25]. In contrast, studies that include individuals in their 8th, 9th, and 10th decade of life have largely either failed to find such an association [26,27] or have found high BP to be protective against cognitive impairment [28,29]. Together, these results suggest that the relationship between cognition and BP in late-life may be age dependent [30].
Inverted U- or J-shaped curves may most accurately represent the
relationship between BP and cognition among octogenarians and
nonagenarians, as both low BP and extremely high BP (systolic blood
pressure (SBP) >160mmHg) have been linked to cognitive impairment in
this age group [28,31–33].
Table 2.
Study | Study Design Duration n | Age at BP Assessment | BP Measure | Age at Cognitive Assessment | Cognitive Domains Assessed | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
COGNIPRES, Spain [21] | Cross-sectional n = 1,579 | 71 (7) | HTN, SBP, DBP, PP, MAP | 71 (7) | MMSE | HTN and antihypertensive medication noncompliance were associated with lower MMSE scores. |
Kungsholmen Project, Sweden [203] | Longitudinal 3 years n = 1,736 | 75-101 | SBP, DBP | T1: 75-101 T2: 78-104 | MMSE | Higher SBP and DBP at baseline were associated with better MMSE score at baseline and 3-year follow-up. Baseline SBP < 130 mmHg was associated with increased risk of cognitive impairment at follow-up. |
REGARDS, USA [204] | Cross-sectional n = 14,566 | 65 (9) | HTN | 65 (9) | Six-item Screener | HTN was not associated with risk of cognitive impairment. |
Framingham Heart Study, USA [23] | Cross-temporal a 13 years n = 1,702 | 67 (8) | HTN | 80 | Abstract reasoning Language Memory Verbal fluency Working memory | Baseline HTN was associated with poorer working memory, visual memory, and verbal memory at follow-up among participants not on antihypertensive medication. |
Framingham Heart Study, USA [205] | Cross-temporal a 4-6 years n = 1,423 | 66 (7) | HTN | 71 | Abstract reasoning Executive function Memory | Baseline HTN was associated with greater memory impairment 4-6 years later in men, but not in women. |
Northern Manhattan Study, USA [25] | Longitudinal 6 years n = 4,337 | 76 (6) | HTN | T1: 78 T2: 80 T3: 81 | Executive function Language Memory | Baseline HTN was associated with declines in executive functioning, but not memory or language. |
REGARDS USA [22] | Cross-sectional n = 19,836 | 65 (10) | SBP, DBP, PP | 65 (10) | Six-item Screener | Higher DBP was associated with greater risk of cognitive impairment. |
Indo-US Cross National Dementia Epidemiology Study, India/USA [27] | Cross-sectional n = 4810 n = 636 | 67 (7) 82 (4) | SBP, DBP | 67 (7) 82 (4) | MMSE | Higher SBP and DBP were associated with increased risk for cognitive impairment in the younger Indian cohort. No association between BP and cognitive impairment was found in the older American cohort. |
Osservatorio Geriatrico Regione Campania, Italy [206] | Cross-sectional n = 1,229 | 74 (6) | SBP, DBP | 74 (6) | MMSE | Higher DBP, but not SBP, was associated with cognitive impairment in participants > 75, but not in participants 65-74 years of age. |
Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Ageing, USA [29] | Longitudinal 11 years n = 847 | 71 (9) | SBP, DBP | 82 | Attention Executive function Memory Naming Processing speed | High SBP was associated with memory declines among older participants. Cross-sectional analyses demonstrated that both high and low diastolic BP were associated with poorer executive functioning, processing speed, and naming among participant groups. |
Chicago Health and Aging, USA [28] | Cross-sectional n = 5,816 | 65-104 | SBP, DBP | 65-104 | MMSE | Participants with SBP < 100 mm Hg and SBP > 140 mm Hg had lower MMSE scores. |
Chicago Health and Aging, USA [207] | Longitudinal 6 years n = 4,284 | 74 (6) | SBP, DBP | 80 | MMSE Memory Processing speed | BP was not associated with cognitive change over the span of 6 years. |
The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging, Italy [208] | Cross-sectional n = 3,425 | 65-84 | HTN | 65-84 | MMSE | Hypertension was not associated with MMSE score. |
Men Born in 1914, Sweden [209] | Cross-sectional n = 500 | 68 | HTN | 68 | Processing speed Verbal abilities Visual memory Visuospatial | HTN (SBP 140-159 mm Hg) was associated with better visuospatial and verbal abilities. Severe HTN (SBP ≥ 180 mm Hg) was associated with poorer performance on measures of memory and processing speed. |
Cardiovascular Health Study, USA [210] | Longitudinal 7 years n = 5,888 | ≥ 65 | SBP | ≥ 72 | Modified MMSE Processing speed | SBP Elevated SBP was associated with a decline in MMSE and processing speed over a period of 7 years. |
East Boston cohort study, USA [32] | Longitudinal 9 years n = 3,657 | 74 (6) | SBP, DBP | 83 | Memory SPMSQ | A U-shaped relationship between SBP and cognition was found whereby SBP < 130mm Hg or ≥ 160mm Hg was associated with a higher rate of errors on a mental status questionnaire (SPMSQ). |
Duke Population Studies of the Elderly, USA [33] | Longitudinal 3 years n = 3,202 | 73 (6) | SBP, DBP | 76 | SPMSQ | Among white participants, a U-shaped relationship between SBP cognitive decline was found whereby SBP < 110 mm Hg and SBP > 165 mm Hg was associated with 3-year cognitive decline. No association between BP and cognition was found in black participants. |
East Boston Study, USA [26] | Cross-sectional n = 3,627 | ≥ 65 | SBP, DBP, HTN | ≥ 65 | Attention Memory | BP was not associated with cognitive functioning. |
BP
= blood pressure; COGNIPRES = Cognitive function and blood pressure
control; DBP = diastolic blood pressure; HTN = hypertension; MAP = mean
arterial presure; MMSE = Mini Mental Status Exam; PP = pulse pressure;
REGARDS = Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke; SBP =
systolic blood pressure; SPMSQ = Short Portable Mental Status
Questionnaire
aCross temporal: a
study design in which the exposure variable (e.g., hypertension) and the
outcome variable (e.g., cognition) are measured distict time points.
While
individuals who develop hypertension earlier in life are likely to be
subjected to the deleterious neurological effects of hypertension for
many decades, this is not the case for individuals who develop
hypertension much later. The strong associations found between midlife
hypertension and late-life cognitive abilities supports the notion that
hypertension duration and chronicity in adulthood may be especially
important determinants of cognitive impairment in elderly individuals.
Perhaps the strongest support for this hypothesis comes from a
longitudinal study which found that a longer duration of time between
hypertension initiation and cognitive testing is associated with reduced
cognitive abilities independent of age [18].
In particular, longitudinal studies suggest that middle-aged adults
with prolonged hypertension and elevated systolic blood pressure (SBP)
over a period of 25–30 years are at an exceptionally high risk for
cognitive impairment later in life [17,18].
Thus, studies with a longer period between the initiation of BP
monitoring and subsequent cognitive assessment may be better able to
detect the effects of high BP on neurocognitive outcomes. The trajectory
of blood pressure changes from midlife into older age may also be
important, as the combination of hypertension in midlife and low
diastolic blood pressure (DBP) in late-life has been associated with
smaller brain volumes and poorer cognitive outcomes among older adults [34].
Individuals who develop hypertension before middle adulthood may also
be at particularly high risk for cognitive impairment, as a number of
studies have found associations between high BP, cognitive deficits, and
reduced academic functioning in children, adolescents, and young adults
[35–40].
Irrespective of age, the cognitive domains that appear most vulnerable
to hypertension are executive functioning and information processing
speed. Both cognitive processes rely heavily on the integrity of frontal
and subcortical brain structures which may be most vulnerable to the
effects of hypertension.
Blood pressure variability
BP
fluctuates substantially over a 24-hour period as a result of factors
such as postural change, circadian rhythm, and general physiologic
variability [41,42].
Fluctuations in BP associated with autonomic dysfunction, such as
orthostatic hypotension, become more prevalent with increasing age and
may be associated with cognitive deficits [41,43,44].
Although a number of studies have demonstrated a connection between
orthostatic hypotension and cognitive function, with worse performance
in the setting of orthostasis [45–48], others have failed to replicate this finding [49–51].
Ambulatory blood pressure measurement (ABPM) has been used in a number
of studies to more accurately capture short-term, daily BP variability,
which may reflect autonomic dysfunction or increased arterial stiffness,
among other etiologies. Using ABPM, elevated 24-hour mean BP, 24-hour
BP variability, and reduced nocturnal dipping (a natural reduction of
night-time BP) have each been identified as potential risk factors for
cognitive impairment [11,52–54]. Because autonomic dysfunction occurs in the early phase of several neurodegenerative disorders [55],
it is difficult to determine whether cognitive deficits found in
individuals with potential sequelae of autonomic dysfunction (e.g., BP
variability and orthostatic hypotension) are the result of underlying
neurodegenerative changes or the direct effect of transient drops in BP.
Genetic factors
Additional
insights into the relationship between hypertension and cognition have
emerged through genetic studies. A polymorphism in the ACE gene, a gene which regulates BP through its effects on angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) activity [56], has been linked to both cognitive function [57] and the presence of neuroimaging abnormalities [58,59].
Middle-aged and older adults who carry an allele that codes for the
high activity variant (D) of the ACE I/D polymorphism show greater
levels of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline [57,60–62].
Unexpectedly, other studies have found the low activity allele (I) of
the ACE I/D polymorphism to confer increased risk for dementia [63,64].
Polymorphisms in another gene, AGTR1, which codes for the
angiotensin-II type 1 receptor, also an important part of the regulation
of BP, have been associated with reduced prefrontal and hippocampal
volume [65], reductions in hippocampal volume over time, and poorer memory in older adults [66].
Additional evidence suggests that specific genetic variants may
interact with hypertension to promote or buffer against the effects of
elevated BP on cognitive function and brain structural integrity. Two
Alzheimer’s disease risk genes that have also been associated with
cognitive function in nondemented individuals, Apolipoprotein E (APOE) and Clusterin (CLU), appear to modify the effect of hypertension on cognitive function [67].
For example, multiple studies have found that hypertension is only
associated with cognitive deficits in individuals who possess a copy of
the ε4 allele of the APOE gene [68,69].
Dementia risk and hypertension
Alzheimer’s disease
Several
forms of cardiovascular disease have been identified as risk factors
for both Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia [70–73], which together account for the majority of dementia cases worldwide [74,75]. Alzheimer’s disease, cerebrovascular disease, and cardiovascular disease have shared genetic contributions [76,77],
and approximately 50% of individuals diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease
display significant cerebrovascular pathology on autopsy [78,79].
Together, these findings suggest that cardiovascular disease,
Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia may have an overlapping
pathophysiology [80–82].
Despite
significant evidence for the role of cardiovascular disease in the
pathogenesis and progression of Alzheimer’s disease, the association
between hypertension and Alzheimer’s disease is still not well
understood. Although a consistent relationship between elevated DBP at
midlife and incident Alzheimer’s disease has been demonstrated [7,83,84], evidence for an association between midlife SBP and incident Alzheimer’s disease has been conflicting [84–88]. What is clear is that late-life hypertension does not appear to be a risk factor for incident Alzheimer’s disease [72,88–93]. In fact, multiple studies suggest that abnormally low DBP in late-life may increase one’s risk for Alzheimer’s disease [91,94–98].
Some, but not all, have argued that this inverse relationship between
late-life DBP and Alzheimer’s disease risk results from a tendency for
BP to decline concurrently with the onset and progression of dementia [90,99,100].
Together, previous findings suggest that the combination of high BP in
midlife followed by low BP in late-life may place individuals at
especially high risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. However, few
studies have examined this hypothesis directly [101].
Vascular dementia
Because hypertension is a known risk factor for cerebral small vessel disease [102] and stroke [4],
hypertension is often considered a risk factor for vascular dementia, a
form of cognitive decline resulting from small- or large-vessel
cerebrovascular disease [9,103].
However, only a handful of studies have directly examined the
relationship between hypertension and vascular dementia. Although
previous research supports the relationship between midlife hypertension
and the development of vascular dementia [8,83,85,104–106],
it is unclear whether there is an association between late-life
hypertension and vascular dementia, as findings have thus far been
conflicting [72,89,91,107].
Compared to the associations between midlife hypertension and incident
Alzheimer’s disease, the associations found between midlife hypertension
and incident vascular dementia tend to be more robust and consistent [8,85,108].
However, because patients are more likely to develop mixed Alzheimer’s
and vascular dementia than pure forms of one or the other, this
distinction may not be meaningful.
Pathophysiology of hypertension as it relates to cognitive decline
Evidence from neuroimaging and biomarker studies
Neuroimaging
has played a pivotal role in advancing the understanding of how BP
influences cognitive function and underlying brain structure. Results
from studies that have examined the relationship between BP and brain
volume are largely consistent with findings from the BP and cognition
studies. High SBP has been associated with smaller regional and total
brain volumes [109–113] and reductions in brain volume over time [114]. The relationship between high DBP and brain volume is less consistent, however [110,112,113,115]. In elderly populations, low SBP [116,117] and low DBP [117,118]
have been associated with reduced brain volume and cortical thickness,
suggesting that the relationship between BP and brain volume may
age-dependent [15,119,120]. A pattern of hypertension in midlife followed by hypotension in late-life appears to be especially harmful [34], particularly in brain regions affected in the earliest phase of Alzheimer’s disease [121].
An
association between hypertension and the development of Alzheimer’s
disease has also been supported by research that examines Alzheimer’s
disease biomarkers directly. Compared to the brains of normotensive
individuals, the brains of individuals with a history of hypertension
show greater levels of β-amyloid plaques, atrophy, and neurofibrillary
tangles [86,122]. Similarly, hypertension has been identified as a risk factor for cortical fibrillar β-amyloid deposits [81,123,124] and reduced glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease-specific brain regions [123,125]
using positron emission tomography (PET) in the brains of cognitively
normal middle-aged and older adults. Consistent with these findings, one
study found that individuals with abnormal plasma β-amyloid levels and
elevated BP at midlife have an especially high risk of developing
Alzheimer’s disease later in life [7].
Hypertension
has also been associated with several defining features of vascular
dementia and cerebral small vessel disease, including WMH volume [15,102], WMH progression [126,127], lacunar infarcts, and cerebral microbleeds [5,128–130]. Supporting the relationship between high BP and white matter pathology, findings from observational studies [127] and clinical trials [113,131]
suggest that treatment of hypertension reduces WMH progression. Even
before the development of overt neuroimaging abnormalities, hypertension
appears to be associated with reduced white matter microstructural
integrity in both young and old individuals, suggesting white matter may
be especially vulnerable to the deleterious effects of hypertension [132–135].
Hypertension and vascular remodeling
Emerging
evidence suggests that sustained elevations in BP may cause cerebral
vessel remodeling in a manner which promotes pathological brain changes
and subsequent cognitive decline. To preserve the steady low-pressure
blood supply to the periphery and protect end organ microcirculation
from pulsatile stress associated with hypertension, a rearrangement in
vessel wall material in the form of hypertrophic remodeling of the media
and vascular smooth muscle cells occurs [136–138].
This enlargement in media size causes a reduction in lumen diameter,
leading to increased vascular resistance and vessel wall stiffening [139].
Arterial stiffening, in turn, increases arterial pulse wave velocity
and pulsatile pressure, which over time causes rarefaction of downstream
capillaries and further inward remodeling of vessel walls [140–142]. Hypertension promotes intracranial atherosclerosis in large intracranial arteries [142,143] and arteriolosclerosis in smaller arterioles supplying subcortical white matter and deep gray matter brain structures [144].
Arteriolosclerosis is a process characterized by a loss of tunica media
smooth muscle cells, fibro-hyaline deposits, and thickening of the
vessel wall, resulting in increased microvascular resistance. Because
the brain requires high levels of continuous perfusion throughout
systole and diastole [145], increases in vascular resistance leave cerebral arterioles vulnerable to hypoperfusion when systemic BP is reduced [139,144]. As described below, hypoperfusion has been associated with several neurovascular changes [82], which together may disrupt cognition [146].
Autoregulation and cerebral perfusion
The
brain requires a high volume of consistent blood flow to sustain
adequate perfusion. However, the brain’s ability to maintain steady
low-pressure blood flow in the context of changing systemic BP – a
process known as cerebral autoregulation – can be disrupted as a result
of chronic hypertension [147,148].
After prolonged exposure to high BP and elevated pulsatility, a shift
occurs in the brain’s autoregulatory capacity whereby higher systemic BP
is required to maintain the same level of cerebral perfusion [149].
Hypertension is believed to alter cerebral autoregulation by inducing
changes in arteriole endothelial and vascular smooth muscle cells that
diminish cerebrovascular reactivity [150] and increase myogenic tone, respectively [151].
Not only do these vascular changes shift the cerebral autoregulatory
curve in a manner which reduces resting cerebral blood flow, but the
brain also becomes more susceptible to hypoperfusion during periods of
low systemic BP [152] or during periods of normal BP in chronically hypertensive individuals [153].
These hypertension-induced changes to cerebral autoregulation and
perfusion may explain why individuals with chronic hypertension in
midlife and low BP in late-life show significant reductions in brain
volume [34,121] and greater levels of cognitive deficits [101].
While
ischemia may occur in some cases, the brain is more likely to be
subjected to chronic oligemia (i.e., mild reductions in blood flow) as a
result of hypertension. Chronic oligemia may, in turn, lead to
endothelial dysfunction, acidosis, oxidative stress, and unmet metabolic
energy demands that can impair neuronal function [82,154,155]. Oligemia may also down-regulate the synthesis of proteins necessary for synaptic plasticity and memory formation [155], and promote neuronal tau phosphorylation, β-amyloid oligomerization, and the upregulation of amyloidogenic APP [156–160].
Each of these neurophysiological changes likely contributes to the
development of Alzheimer’s disease and cerebral amyloid angiopathy
(CAA). Evidence suggests that β-amyloid accumulation may also occur as a
result of hypertension-induced up-regulation of the receptor for
advanced glycation end products (RAGE), which controls the shuttling of
β-amyloid from the blood across the endothelial barrier into the brain [161].
Endothelial dysfunction, altered functional hyperemia & Oxidative stress
By
promoting endothelial dysfunction, hypertension is also believed to
disrupt the coordinated coupling among neurons, glia, and cerebral blood
flow in the microvasculature [162].
Uncoupling of this system, known collectively as the neurovascular
unit, can impair the homeostatic process of functional hyperemia,
whereby increases in CBF occur in coordination with increases in
neuronal activity to ensure the delivery of adequate levels oxygen and
glucose and facilitate the removal of metabolites [163–165].
Support for these findings comes from animal research, which has
demonstrated that hypertension-induced vascular oxidative stress
resulting from up-regulation of reactive oxygen species (ROS)-producing
enzyme NADPH oxidase impairs the endothelium-dependent expression of
vasodilators and vasoconstrictors necessary to maintain neurovascular
coupling [150,166,167].
Antihypertensive clinical trials to improve cognition
Given the apparent association between BP and cognitive function, efforts have been made to determine whether improved BP control can be used to delay cognitive decline and reduce dementia risk. To date, evidence from large placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trials (RCTs) has been conflicting [168,169]. A 2009 Cochrane Review of randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials concluded that there is currently no convincing evidence for the protective effects of antihypertensive use in late-life [169]. Although several large placebo-controlled RCTs, such as the Perindopril Protection Against Recurrent Stroke Study (PROGRESS) [170], the Systolic Hypertension in Europe (SYST-EUR study) [171], and the Heart Outcomes Prevention Evaluation (HOPE) study [172] have found antihypertensive agents to be protective against cognitive decline and dementia, just as many trials have failed to replicate this finding [173–177]. Thus, it is unknown whether BP control alone is enough to reduce the risk of cognitive decline. It is possible that the neuroprotective effects of antihypertensive agents may result from drug-specific neurobiological changes as opposed to (or in addition to) BP lowering [178,179]. In support of this idea, a meta-analysis of RCTs which compared the neuroprotective properties of different antihypertensive drug classes found angiotensin receptor blockers (ARBs) to be superior to β-blockers, diuretics, and ACE inhibitors for preventing cognitive decline [180].
The
ability to draw conclusions about the effectiveness of BP interventions
for the reduction of cognitive decline has been limited by brief study
durations and insufficient power to detect effects. Cognitive decline,
even in the course of neurodegenerative disease, is a relatively gradual
process, and, as described above, elevated BP in midlife may be the
most important determinant of risk for subsequent cognitive and decline
and dementia. Thus, midlife may be the most critical window during which
BP control must begin. Extended treatment and follow-up periods and
larger sample sizes will likely be needed to reliably detect the effects
of BP lowering on cognitive measures. By comparison, neurodegenerative
and dementia-specific biomarkers (e.g., hippocampal atrophy and CSF-tau)
may be more sensitive to treatment-related effects, but their validity
as intermediate endpoints remains a subject of debate [181,182].
Future studies may also benefit from making use of a more comprehensive
cognitive battery. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), which has
been used to assess cognitive abilities in the majority of previous
trials, is notoriously insensitive to cognitive change, especially in
domains of executive functioning and processing speed, making it an
especially poor choice for detecting cognitive change in this context [183,184].
Additionally, effect sizes in previous BP lowering trials may have been
attenuated because participants receiving antihypertensive medication
often saw only minor reductions in BP compared to participants given
placebo. This limitation is addressed in an ongoing trial (SPRINT-MIND)
to evaluate the neuroprotective effects of reducing BP to below a
specific level (i.e., below 120mm Hg) using one or more antihypertensive
agent [185].
The parent trial to this study (SPRINT) has already demonstrated
improved cardiovascular outcomes in the setting of this tighter blood
pressure control [186]. However, the ability of this trial to show benefit in cognitive outcomes will be limited by short follow-up.
Conclusions and future directions
It
is clear that hypertension can affect brain structure and function in a
manner that increases one’s risk of cognitive decline and dementia.
Hypertension, high SBP, and high DBP during midlife have been most
consistently linked to late-life cognitive decline and incident
dementia. However, hypertension has been associated with early-life and
midlife cognitive deficits as well. Although the association between
late-life hypertension and cognitive function is less clear,
particularly among octogenarians and nonagenarians, limited evidence
suggests that mildly elevated BP in late life may be protective against
cognitive decline, especially for individuals with a history of
longstanding hypertension. Hypertension duration may be an especially
important determinant of cognitive decline, as evidence suggests that
the damaging neurological effects of hypertension may be cumulative. Few
studies have assessed BP longitudinally, and even fewer have attempted
to retrospectively determine how lifetime duration of hypertension
relates to cognitive function. Given the increasing prevalence of
hypertension among younger individuals [187],
assessing the cumulative effects of elevated BP over the lifespan will
be especially important to understanding how BP may influence
neurodevelopment and neurodegeneration [188].
Recent
advances in neuroimaging and physiologic and hemodynamic monitoring
have allowed for an improved understanding of the mechanisms through
which hypertension affects neurocognitive function. Hypertension,
especially in midlife, has been identified as a risk factor for cerebral
atrophy, white matter microstructural damage, and cerebral small vessel
disease. Evidence suggests that hypertension contributes to the
development and progression of such neurological changes by promoting
vessel wall remodeling and endothelial dysfunction, which results in
autoregulatory deficits. These changes to the neurovascular unit leave
the brain vulnerable to hypoperfusion resulting from drops in systemic
BP. Although evidence exists to support this model of
hypertension-induced cerebrovascular changes, much is still unknown
about how these pathophysiological processes directly influence
cognitive function and promote Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia in
humans.
Additional insights into the role circulatory
changes play in cognitive decline will likely come from the study of
other markers of vessel function. For example, pulse pressure, a measure
of arterial stiffening, which increases with age and exposure to
hypertension [145],
can be used as an additional method to quantify the effects of vascular
pathology resulting from chronic hypertension. Elevations in pulse
pressure have been associated with cognitive impairment [189,190], cognitive decline [190], cerebral small vessel disease [127,191], and Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers [192].
Compared to BP, pulse pressure is believed to more precisely quantify
the exposure of target organs such as the brain to potentially harmful
pulsatile energy resulting from arterial stiffening [193].
A
more nuanced understanding of the relationship between BP and neural
function will likely be needed before antihypertensive therapies can be
effectively employed as an intervention to reduce cognitive decline.
Given that many individuals who develop hypertension do so before
late-life and experience the harmful effects of hypertension for
decades, it is unclear whether specific antihypertensive agents will be
able to modify the trajectory of cognitive decline within the span of a
multi-year trial. If the effects of hypertension on the brain are
cumulative, interindividual differences in the duration and severity of
previous hypertension must be considered in future trial design. Because
the effects of BP on cognition appear to differ with age, future
clinical trials may also benefit from limiting enrollment to specific
age groups. Other factors such as race, sex, genetics, and the presence
of cerebrovascular morbidity have each been identified as effect
modifiers in observational studies and should, therefore, be considered
when designing future antihypertensive trials.
No comments:
Post a Comment