http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00547/full?
- 1Institute of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
- 2Department of Neurology, Klinik Bavaria Bad Kissingen, Bad Kissingen, Germany
Introduction
Brain-computer interfacing (BCI) does not require motor
control, but instead either willful brain activation or attention
allocation to certain stimuli (Wolpaw and Wolpaw, 2012). Successful BCI use was reported in severely motor-impaired but cognitively intact patients (Hoffmann et al., 2008; Silvoni et al., 2009; Nijboer et al., 2010).
One possible BCI input signal is the P300 which represents a positive
deflection in the EEG occurring 300 ms after the onset of a relevant
stimulus, or target, presented within a stream of irrelevant stimuli, or
non-targets (oddball paradigm, Sutton et al., 1965). In a classic visual P300 spelling paradigm (Farwell and Donchin, 1988),
letters of the alphabet are arranged in a matrix of rows and columns.
The target stimulus is the letter to be selected which is highlighted
(flashed) once in the row and once in the column accordingly. The
non-target stimuli are to be ignored by the BCI user. The BCI detects
the P300 response to the target stimulus cell, displays the target
letter on a computer screen and thereby allows for communication (Farwell and Donchin, 1988).
Even though establishing communication in paralyzed
patients has been one of the major goals of BCI research for decades,
BCIs were most recently also used in rehabilitation contexts (Daly and Huggins, 2015) including rehabilitation after stroke. Morone et al. (2015)
reported on the use of BCI technology for rehabilitation of the upper
limb by having patients undergo a BCI based training schedule. Patients
had to imagine movements with their paralyzed arms and hands and these
imagined movements were translated into movements of a virtual hand. The
authors hypothesized this training to boost neuronal plasticity after
stroke and thereby support motor rehabilitation. And indeed, patients
improved their ability to move hands and arms (as measured by the
Fugl-Meyr scale) to a clinically relevant extent.
Besides possible motor impairments, manifold cognitive impairments might occur after stroke (Jokinen et al., 2015). Up to 50% of all stroke survivors are affected by attention impairments (Leśniak et al., 2008) and up to 30% suffer from language production or language comprehension deficits (= aphasia, Flowers et al., 2013).
In the pilot study presented here, we aimed at focusing on aphasia
caused by lesions in the opercular and triangular part of the inferior
frontal gyrus, the Broca area, the temporoparietal region and related
circuits of the brain (motor aphasia e.g., Berndt and Caramazza, 1999).
Usually, afferent fibers receive information from the primary and
secondary auditory cortices and several association fields. Efferent
fibers to the precentral gyrus are activated directly via the basal
ganglia and indirectly via the thalamus and cerebellum (Trepel, 2011).
Corticonuclear fibers activate nuclei in the brainstem, which initiate
muscle activation in the larynx and the pharynx. Mimics and language are
produced. With motor aphasia, language can still be perceived and
understood, but language production is limited or impossible. In case
language is not completely lost, sentences are short, word production is
flawed and self-expression is very demanding (Kelly et al., 2010).
Language therapy as provided by the healthcare system was shown to have positive effects mainly in early rehabilitation phases (Robey, 1994) and for patients who suffered from language comprehension disorders (Kelly et al., 2010).
Therefore, traditional language therapy is highly valuable, but
unfortunately might not be sufficient. Half of the aphasic patients do
not recover fully (Hartje and Poeck, 2000). In the long term, not only might the psychological burden become manifest as a consequence of chronic aphasia (Gainotti, 1997), but also the economic situation might deteriorate (Hinckley, 1998)
as in many workplaces employees must be able to communicate
elaborately. Therefore, aphasia rehabilitation after stroke is a
challenge deserving more attention by research and rehabilitation care.
BCI use for communication was tested and reported successful in eight participants diagnosed with aphasia (Shih et al., 2014).
The authors used a visual P300 based checkerboard paradigm and reported
achieved accuracies of between 60% and 65%. Another approach was
presented most recently by Musso et al. (2016)
who presented an auditory BCI system being used by a patient with
chronic aphasia. Their goal was to identify neuronal markers of auditory
attention. However, in both mentioned studies, the theoretical
background for BCI use and therefore, attention allocation which might
be useful in aphasia rehabilitation, were missing. To our knowledge, the
presented relation between attention, aphasia and BCI use and its
potential implications for aphasia rehabilitation are the first
theory-based considerations on this research topic.
More at link.
More at link.
No comments:
Post a Comment